0
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Original Contribution |

Association of Life Activities With Cerebral Blood Flow in Alzheimer Disease:  Implications for the Cognitive Reserve Hypothesis FREE

Nikolaos Scarmeas, MD; Eric Zarahn, PhD; Karen E. Anderson, MD; Christian G. Habeck, PhD; John Hilton, PhD; Joseph Flynn; Karen S. Marder, MD, MPH; Karen L. Bell, MD; Harold A. Sackeim, PhD; Ronald L. Van Heertum, MD; James R. Moeller, PhD; Yaakov Stern, PhD
[+] Author Affiliations

From the Cognitive Neuroscience Division of the Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer's Disease and the Aging Brain (Drs Scarmeas, Habeck, Hilton, and Stern, and Mr Flynn); and the Departments of Neurology (Drs Scarmeas, Marder, Bell, and Stern), Psychiatry (Drs Zarahn, Anderson, Sackeim, Moeller, and Stern), and Radiology (Drs Sackeim and Van Heertum), College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University, New York, NY.


Arch Neurol. 2003;60(3):359-365. doi:10.1001/archneur.60.3.359.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Background  Regional cerebral blood flow (CBF), a good indirect index of cerebral pathologic changes in Alzheimer disease (AD), is more severely reduced in patients with higher educational attainment and IQ when controlling for clinical severity. This has been interpreted as suggesting that cognitive reserve allows these patients to cope better with the pathologic changes in AD.

Objective  To evaluate whether premorbid engagement in various activities may also provide cognitive reserve.

Design  We evaluated intellectual, social, and physical activities in 9 patients with early AD and 16 healthy elderly controls who underwent brain H215O positron emission tomography. In voxelwise multiple regression analyses that controlled for age and clinical severity, we investigated the association between education, estimated premorbid IQ, and activities, and CBF.

Results  In accordance with previous findings, we replicated an inverse association between education and CBF and IQ and CBF in patients with AD. In addition, there was a negative correlation between previous reported activity score and CBF in patients with AD. When both education and IQ were added as covariates in the same model, a higher activity score was still associated with more prominent CBF deficits. No significant associations were detected in the controls.

Conclusions  At any given level of clinical disease severity, there is a greater degree of brain pathologic involvement in patients with AD who have more engagement in activities, even when education and IQ are taken into account. This may suggest that interindividual differences in lifestyle may affect cognitive reserve by partially mediating the relationship between brain damage and the clinical manifestation of AD.

Figures in this Article

THE COGNITIVE reserve (CR) hypothesis suggests that there are individual differences in the ability to cope with the pathologic changes in Alzheimer disease (AD).1 Innate intelligence or aspects of life experience may supply reserve in the form of a set of skills or repertoires that allow some people to cope with the pathologic changes better than others. Educational and occupational attainments are considered such aspects of life experience.

Epidemiologic data supporting the CR hypothesis include observations that higher educational and occupational attainment is associated with decreased risk for incident dementia.2 Functional imaging studies have also provided support for the concept of CR. Considering cerebral blood flow (CBF) as an indirect index of pathologic changes in disease310 (lower blood flow indicating more advanced pathologic changes in AD) studies have shown that patients with higher educational11 or occupational12 attainment as well as those with a higher premorbid IQ13 have more prominent flow deficits when controlling for clinical severity. These flow deficits were located in the brain regions typically associated with reduced CBF in AD. Again, these observations support the prediction that individuals with more CR can tolerate more pathologic changes. Factors other than education and occupation might also provide reserve against pathologic changes in AD. Both cross-sectional and prospective longitudinal studies have suggested that engaging in various social, intellectual, and leisure activities is associated with reduced risk of prevalent or incident AD.1418

The present study was designed to use the functional imaging approach just described to clarify the role of reported activities in CR. We evaluated intellectual, social, and physical activities in patients with AD who underwent brain H215O positron emission tomography (PET). A role for such activities in CR would predict that, when controlling for disease severity, patients with higher activity scores would have more advanced pathologic changes in AD. Thus, using CBF as an indirect indicator of pathologic changes in disease would show a negative correlation between activity scores and CBF in areas of the brain that typically show reduced CBF in AD.

SUBJECTS

Nine patients who met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition (DSM-III-R) criteria for dementia19 and the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association for probable AD20 and 16 healthy elderly controls participated in this study. The patients underwent extensive neuropsychologic evaluation, including the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised,21 the American version of the Nelson Adult Reading Test (NART) estimated IQ,2224 the modified Mini-Mental State examination (mMMS),2527 and tests of memory (short- and long-term verbal28 and nonverbal28,29), orientation, abstract reasoning (verbal21 and nonverbal30), language (naming,31 verbal fluency,32,33 comprehension,33 and repetition33), and construction (copying34 and matching29). In addition, the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale (part I, sections A and B)35 was administered. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was used to rule out patients with vascular diseases or tumors. Other causes of dementia were excluded with appropriate laboratory tests. The diagnosis of AD was reached at a consensus diagnostic conference of physicians and neuropsychologists. Positron emission tomography results did not play any role in the diagnostic process.

ACTIVITIES

Before the PET scan, an interview with the patient and the informant or the healthy subject assessed activities engaged in during the last 6 months. The questionnaire was an expanded version of a scale administered to a community of 1772 elderly subjects in a prospective incidence dementia study.18 Participation in 18 activities was recorded (Table 1). The sum of the points over the 18 activities was calculated for each patient and this leisure score was used as a predictor in subsequent analyses. It was also recorded whether the amount of time patients had spent doing each activity had decreased, remained the same, or increased during the previous 10 years.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1 Items Included in the Activities Scale
PET SCAN ACQUISITION AND PROCESSING

Scans were collected while the subject was at rest with eyes closed. For each scan, a bolus of 30 mCi (1110 MBq) of intravenous H215O was injected. Using an EXACT 47 PET camera (Siemens, Knoxville, Tenn), three 30-second scan frames were acquired in 2-dimensional mode beginning 20 seconds after tracer administration. After measured attenuation correction (15-minute transmission scan) and reconstruction by filtered back-projection, image resolution was 10 mm full width at half-maximum (FWHM). Arterial blood sampling was not conducted; thus, nonquantitative count images (and not absolute CBF measures) were obtained.

Using modules from the statistical parametric mapping program (SPM99; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, England), the following steps were performed in turn for each subject: (1) The 3 PET frames were realigned to each other and summed. (2) A T1-weighted MRI was coregistered to this summed PET image. Magnetic resonance images were acquired using a 1.5-Tesla MR scanner. A T1 3-dimensional spoiled-gradient-recalled echo sequence (repetition time [TR] = 34 ms; echo time [TE] = 5 ms; flip angle = 45°) was used to acquire T1-weighted images with an in-plane resolution of 0.859 mm × 0.859 mm (256 × 256 matrix; 22 cm2 field of view). One hundred twenty four –1.5-mm transaxial slices were acquired. The z-dimension was down-sampled to a final dimension of 6 mm per slice. (3) The coregistered MRI was spatially transformed to the coordinates defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute template brain provided by SPM99. (4) The spatial normalization parameters were applied to the summed PET image, which was resliced using sinc interpolation to a final voxel size of 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm. (5) The spatially normalized PET image was intensity normalized by its average perirolandic count value (given that both pathologic and quantitative PET CBF imaging data suggest that the perirolandic cortex is typically spared by pathologic changes in disease8,10,36,37). (6) The spatially and intensity normalized PET image was spatially smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 8 mm).

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

Various voxelwise multiple regression analyses38 were performed on the intensity normalized images (subsequently referred to as "CBF" with the understanding that they are not in physiologic units). Values of the CBF were used as the dependent variable in all of the multiple regression models.

Several regression models were separately estimated. Putative CR variables in these various models included education, estimates of premorbid IQ, and the activity score. Word-reading ability as assessed by NART was used to estimate premorbid IQ. Performance on this task remains relatively preserved in mild AD and is considered a good estimate of premorbid abilities.39 The score on the mMMS was used as an index of the clinical severity of dementia.25,26,40

Our strategy for the regression analyses was to first assess the association between CBF and each putative CR variable individually. We controlled for age41 and mMMS score (ie, clinical severity) in all of the models. We first introduced years of education and NART score (in 2 separate models) as independent variables to replicate previous findings. We then used the activity score as an independent variable in a separate model. We finally sought to explore whether there was a relationship between CBF and activity score while accounting for the other variables (education and NART score).

For each regression model, voxelwise relationships between the scaled count values and the independent variables of interest were calculated and statistically assessed with t values.38 The false-positive rate was controlled at α = .05 per map via Bonferroni correction for the number of statistically independent resolution elements (resels) across which regressions were calculated (number of resels = total volume of map / product of FWHM across x, y, and z dimensions).42

We used the tissue density values from subjects' MRI results (in voxels where significant associations were noted in the previous analyses) as covariates in the regression analyses to explore the possibility that any associations between the CR variables and CBF are mediated by atrophy. The atrophy analyses did not change the results.

DEMOGRAPHICS, LEISURE SCORES, AND CR SUMMARY MEASURES

Demographic, clinical, and neuropsychologic variables for each of the AD subjects are presented in Table 2. The same data are presented for both patients with AD and controls in Table 3. There were 5 men and 4 women in the AD group and 6 men and 10 women in the control group.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2 Demographic, Clinical, and Neuropsychologic Data for 9 Patients With Alzheimer Disease
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3 Demographic, Cognitive, and Functional Characteristics of the Subjects*

Although all of the test subjects had only mild AD, it could be argued that current activities do not accurately reflect premorbid lifestyle. We therefore examined the reported change in the amount of time spent doing each activity during the last 10 years. For 5 patients, the time spent on each activity was reported to have either increased or remained stable over the last 10 years. Four patients reported decreased time devoted to 2 of 18, 3 of 18, 4 of 18, and 6 of 18 activities, respectively. Since these activities represented only a small fraction of the overall score, with most of the activities exercised at a stable rate or more frequently, we considered the activity score to be a reasonable estimate of activities, at least during the decade preceding the study.

Correlations between education and NART score were r = 0.59, P<.002 (AD: r = 0.60, P<.09; control: r = 0.67, P<.004); between education and activities, r = 0.29, P<.15 (AD: r = 0.60, P<.08; control: r = −0.15, P<.59); and between NART score and activities, r = 0.53, P<.007 (AD: r = 0.81, P<.008; control: r = −0.05, P<.85). The nonsignificant correlations may indicate that the variables are not truly associated. Nevertheless, it is hard to draw many conclusions from this given the very small number of subjects (9 patients with AD and 16 controls), which may have resulted in low power to detect existing significant correlations.

REGRESSION ANALYSES

In accordance with previous findings,11 education was inversely associated with CBF (Table 4). Similarly, in line with previous reports,13 there was a negative association between premorbid IQ (as measured by the NART) and CBF (Table 4).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 4 Local Maxima With Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow (CBF) and Education and Between CBF and Nelson Adult Reading Test Score*

The activity score was also inversely correlated with CBF when controlling for age and mMMS score (Table 5 and Figure 1). Significant associations were localized mainly to the temporal lobe but also in temporal-parietal-occipital association areas. When simultaneously controlling for age, mMMS score, education, and NART score, activity score was still negatively correlated with CBF (Table 6).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 5 Local Maxima With a Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow and Activity Scores*
Place holder to copy figure label and caption

Statistical parametric map and its 3-dimensional brain rendering representation depicting areas of significant (some P<.001, some P<.01, and some P<.05, as presented in Table 4) inverse correlations between cerebral blood flow and activities score in the Alzheimer group, controlling for age and Mini-Mental State Examination score.

Graphic Jump Location
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 6 Local Maxima With a Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow and Activity Scores*

With the exception of 2 voxels (1 in the cerebellum [Table 5], and 1 in the lentiform nucleus [Table 6]), no significant associations were detected for the controls. We did not detect positive associations in any of the analyses, neither in the AD nor in the control group.

Consistent with the prediction of the CR hypothesis, we found that activities had a negative correlation with CBF in patients with AD when controlling for disease severity. This relationship was seen primarily in voxels located in the temporal but also in temporal-occipital-parietal association cortices, the areas in which CBF changes are typically noted in AD. This inverse association was still present when both education and IQ were included in the model. This indicates that there is a unique relationship between activities and CBF over and above the relationship of CBF to education and IQ.

A previous study noted an inverse association between educational attainment and CBF (controlling for disease severity) in parietal areas.11 In our study, we observed a similar negative correlation between education and CBF, localizing to parietal, frontal, and temporal regions.

Another study found an inverse association between premorbid IQ and CBF in prefrontal, premotor, orbitofrontal, superior parietal, thalamic, and anterior cingulate areas.13 In our study, similar results (in terms of directionality of the relationship between IQ and CBF) were obtained. Significant correlations included thalamic, temporal, parietal, frontal, and cingulate areas in our study.

The observed differences in localization between previous studies and the present one could derive from the different types of functional imaging modalities used (xenon and fluorodeoxyglucose in previous studies vs oxygen 15 here), different covariates reflecting disease severity used in the analysis, different denominators used for intensity normalization, or different measures of IQ. Most notable, however, is the different methods of analysis: both previous studies used a region-of-interest analysis when they examined the associations between education and CBF and IQ and CBF,11,13 while we elected to use a voxelwise analysis. Because we were able to replicate the directionality and, to a certain extent, the localization of these earlier findings with a voxelwise analysis, the validity of these previous observations is strengthened.

We detected significant associations in different areas of the brain depending on whether education or IQ or activities were used in the analyses. At first approximation, this may suggest that different aspects of CR mediate clinical protection in an anatomically specific way: ie, patients with high educational attainment can be maintained at similar clinical severity status when the pathologic changes AD affect certain areas of the brain, while patients with high IQ or leisure activity scores can be maintained when other brain regions are affected. Nevertheless, it is also possible that different aspects of CR are not region-specific and that the locations reported in our analyses generally co-localize to the same extended association areas usually affected by AD. The combination of the limited number of subjects included in our analyses and our conservative approach for controlling for type I error might have limited our power to detect significant associations in the whole spectrum of affected areas of the brain. In addition, the locations reported in our analyses represent only local maxima of more extended areas of the brain (Figure 1). Maintaining intellectual and social engagement through participation in everyday activities seems to buffer healthy individuals against cognitive decline in later life.4345

We recently reported the results of a longitudinal epidemiologic study in northern Manhattan, NY.18 Thirteen leisure activities (intellectual, social, and physical in character) were assessed in 1772 healthy elderly subjects who were prospectively followed for up to 7 years. Even when multiple potentially confounding factors were controlled for, subjects with high leisure activity scores had 38% less risk of developing dementia. There are at least 2 other large prospective cohorts that have reported a protective effect for leisure16 and cognitive activities17 in relationship to incident dementia.

A limitation of the present study is that activities were assessed in patients with mild dementia and that the extent to which they participated in these activities might have been affected by the disease. Optimally, activities should have been recorded during presymptomatic periods. However, as described in the "Results" section, our patients were at early stages of AD and the reported changes in their activities over the last 10 years were ostensibly negligible. We therefore used the recorded activity score as an estimate of lifestyle during the 10 years before study enrollment.

To further explore this potential bias, we repeated the analyses using reconstructed scores for the subjects who reported decreased time devoted to activities, adding in points for the activities they reported to exercise less over the last 10 years. The results for all models were essentially unchanged. We elected to use the contemporaneous activity score for the analyses because it is less prone to recollection bias. The fact that the activities information was corroborated by the informant also added to the face validity of the scores.

The activity scale items collected in this study reflected not only intellectual and social activities but also physical ones. Epidemiologic evidence that physical exercise may delay cognitive impairment is equivocal. While high levels of physical activity were associated with reduced risk of dementia in at least 4 prospective studies,18,4648 no effect of exercise on dementia and cognitive impairment risk was reported from other cohorts.17,49 Additionally, there is basic research evidence that environmental enrichment in the form of voluntary wheel running is associated with enhanced neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus.50 It has also been shown that physical activity sustains cerebral blood flow51 and may improve aerobic capacity and cerebral nutrient supply.52,53 Therefore, although it is conceivable that physical activity may merely be a nonspecific marker of good health indirectly related to dementia (or not related to dementia at all), it is also possible that it has a direct physiologic association with brain disease.

Recent evidence indicates that certain areas of the brain retain the capability to generate new neurons into adulthood, not only in rodents54 and primates55 but also in humans.56,57 Thus, it is possible that the stimulation provided by everyday intellectual and social activities facilitates the maintenance of general cognitive skills in a manner that is analogous to physical exercise for musculoskeletal and cardiovascular functions.43 This reserve could be the result of a physiologic process involving increased synaptic density in the neocortical association cortex acquired by stimulation.58

This study does not indicate that more engagement in activities affords some kind of immunity to the neuropathologic aspect of the AD process. On the contrary, we assume that the pathologic changes in AD progress independently of life activities. The concept of CR was developed in an attempt to explain interindividual differences in the degree of pathologic changes in the brain necessary for the clinical expression of disease.59 This has been confirmed in multiple studies, which have found that a significant proportion of clinically nondemented individuals manifest neuropathologic changes consistent with AD at autopsy.60,61 More engagement in activities may supply a reserve that allows an individual to cope longer before AD is clinically expressed. Aspects of life experience, such as social, intellectual, and physical activities, could modify the paradigms used by the brain to mediate a task by making individuals more efficient or resilient in the face of pathologic changes in the brain or by recruitment of alternate networks.

The activity score may simply represent innate rather than acquired abilities. However, activities, education, and IQ seem to have a unique association to CBF, which supports the concept that aspects of life experience may modulate reserve. The hypothesized contribution of life experiences, styles, and activities to the ability to cope with the pathologic changes in AD suggests the possibility of interventions that might delay the onset of the clinical symptoms of the disease.

Corresponding author and reprints: Nikolaos Scarmeas, MD, Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center, P&S Mailbox 16, PH 19th Floor, 630 W 168th St, New York, NY 10032 (e-mail: ns257@columbia.edu).

Accepted for publication October 23, 2002.

Author contributions: Study concept and design (Drs Scarmeas, Anderson, Sackeim, Van Heertum, and Stern); acquisition of data (Drs Scarmeas, Anderson, Hilton, Marder, Bell, Van Heertum, and Stern, and Mr Flynn); analysis and interpretation of data (Drs Scarmeas, Zarahn, Habeck, Hilton, Sackeim, Van Heertum, Moeller, and Stern, and Mr Flynn); drafting of the manuscript (Drs Scarmeas and Van Heertum); critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content (Drs Zarahn, Anderson, Habeck, Hilton, Marder, Bell, Sackeim, Van Heertum, Moeller, and Stern, and Mr Flynn); statistical expertise (Drs Scarmeas, Zarahn, Habeck, Hilton, Sackeim, Moeller, and Stern, and Mr Flynn); obtained funding (Drs Sackeim and Stern); administrative, technical, and material support (Drs Sackeim, Van Heertum, and Stern); study supervision (Drs Scarmeas, Anderson, Van Heertum, and Stern).

This research was supported by federal grants AG 14671 and RR 00645.

Stern  Y What is cognitive reserve? theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.2002;8:448-460.
Stern  YGurland  BTatemichi  TKTang  MXWilder  DMayeux  R Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. JAMA.1994;271:1004-1010.
McGeer  PLKamo  HHarrop  R  et al Positron emission tomography in patients with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. CMAJ.1986;134:597-607.
McGeer  PLKamo  HHarrop  R  et al Comparison of PET, MRI, and CT with pathology in a proven case of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.1986;36:1569-1574.
McGeer  EGMcGeer  PLHarrop  RAkiyama  HKamo  H Correlations of regional postmortem enzyme activities with premortem local glucose metabolic rates in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci Res.1990;27:612-619.
McGeer  EGPeppard  RPMcGeer  PL  et al 18Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography studies in presumed Alzheimer cases, including 13 serial scans. Can J Neurol Sci.1990;17:1-11.
Mielke  RSchroder  RFink  GRKessler  JHerholz  KHeiss  WD Regional cerebral glucose metabolism and postmortem pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neuropathol.1996;91:174-179.
Hoffman  JMWelsh-Bohmer  KAHanson  M  et al FDG PET imaging in patients with pathologically verified dementia. J Nucl Med.2000;41:1920-1928.
Friedland  RPBrun  ABudinger  TF Pathological and positron emission tomographic correlations in Alzheimer's disease [letter]. Lancet.1985;1:228.
DeCarli  CAtack  JRBall  MJ  et al Post-mortem regional neurofibrillary tangle densities but not senile plaque densities are related to regional cerebral metabolic rates for glucose during life in Alzheimer's disease patients. Neurodegeneration.1992;1:113-121.
Stern  YAlexander  GEProhovnik  IMayeux  R Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol.1992;32:371-375.
Stern  YAlexander  GEProhovnik  I  et al Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: implications for a reserve against Alzheimer's disease pathology. Neurology.1995;45:55-60.
Alexander  GEFurey  MLGrady  CL  et al Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry.1997;154:165-172.
Kondo  KNiino  MShido  K A case-control study of Alzheimer's disease in Japan: significance of life-styles. Dementia.1994;5:314-326.
Friedland  RPFritsch  TSmyth  KA  et al Patients with Alzheimer's disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.2001;98:3440-3445.
Fabrigoule  CLetenneur  LDartigues  JFZarrouk  MCommenges  DBarberger-Gateau  P Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc.1995;43:485-490.
Wilson  RSMendes de Leon  CFBarnes  L  et al Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA.2002;287:742-748.
Scarmeas  NLevy  GTang  MManly  JStern  Y Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.2001;57:2236-2242.
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.  Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987.
McKhann  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology.1984;34:939-944.
Wechsler  D Wechler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised.  New York, NY: The Psychological Corp; 1981.
Grober  ESliwinski  M Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1991;13:933-949.
Nelson  HEO'Connell  A Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex.1978;14:234-244.
Nelson  HE The National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual.  Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson; 1982.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res.1975;12:189-198.
Mayeux  RStern  YRosen  JLeventhal  J Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson disease. Neurology.1981;31:645-650.
Stern  YSano  MPaulson  JMayeux  R Modified mini-mental state examination: validity and reliability. Neurology.1987;37 (suppl 1):179.
Buschke  HFuld  PA Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology.1974;24:1019-1025.
Benton  A The Visual Retention Test.  New York, NY: The Psychological Corp; 1955.
Mattis  S Mental Status ecamination for organic mental syndrome inthe elderly patient.  In: Bellak  L Karasu TB, eds. Geriatric Psychiatry. New York, NY: Grune & Stratton; 1976.
Kaplan  EGoodglass  HWeintraub  S Boston Naming Test.  Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
Benton  AHamsher  A Multiligual Aphasia Examination.  Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1976.
Goodglass  HKaplan  D The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
Not Available The Rosen Drawing Test.  Bronx NY: Veterans Administration Medical Center; 1981.
Blessed  GTomlinson  BERoth  M The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry.1968;114:797-811.
Arnold  SEHyman  BTFlory  JDamasio  ARVan Hoesen  GW The topographical and neuroanatomical distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the cerebral cortex of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex.1991;1:103-116.
Smith  GSde Leon  MJGeorge  AE  et al Topography of cross-sectional and longitudinal glucose metabolic deficits in Alzheimer's disease: pathophysiologic implications. Arch Neurol.1992;49:1142-1150.
Friston  KJHolmes  APWorsley  KJPoline  JPFrackowiak  RSJ Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp.1995;2:189-210.
Lezak  M Neuropsychological Assessment.  New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
Stern  YSano  MPaulson  JMayeux  R Modified mini-mental state examination: validity and reliability. Neurology.1987;37(suppl 1):179.
Cabeza  R Functional neuroimaging of cognitive aging.  In: Cabeza  R, Kingstone  A, eds.Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition. Cambridge, Mass: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press; 2001:331-377.
Worsley  KJMarrett  SNeelin  PVandal  ACFriston  KJEvans  AC A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp.1996;4:58-73.
Hultsch  DFHertzog  CSmall  BJDixon  RA Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging.1999;14:245-263.
Gold  DPAndres  DEtezadi  JArbuckle  TSchwartzman  AChaikelson  J Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. Psychol Aging.1995;10:294-303.
Schaie  K Midlife influences upon intellectual functioning in old age. Int J Behav Dev.1984;7:463-478.
Li  GShen  YCChen  CHZhau  YWLi  SRLu  M A three-year follow-up study of age-related dementia in an urban area of Beijing. Acta Psychiatr Scand.1991;83:99-104.
Yoshitake  TKiyohara  YKato  I  et al Incidence and risk factors of vascular dementia and Alzheimer's disease in a defined elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Neurology.1995;45:1161-1168.
Laurin  DVerreault  RLindsay  JMacPherson  KRockwood  K Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol.2001;58:498-504.
Broe  GACreasey  HJorm  AF  et al Health habits and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: a prospective study on the effects of exercise, smoking and alcohol consumption. Aust N Z J Public Health.1998;22:621-623.
van Praag  HKempermann  GGage  FH Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci.1999;2:266-270.
Rogers  RLMeyer  JSMortel  KF After reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc.1990;38:123-128.
Dustman  RERuhling  RORussell  EM  et al Aerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individuals. Neurobiol Aging.1984;5:35-42.
Spirduso  WW Physical fitness, aging, and psychomotor speed: a review. J Gerontol.1980;35:850-865.
Kempermann  GKuhn  HGGage  FH More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature.1997;386:493-495.
Gould  EReeves  AJGraziano  MSGross  CG Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science.1999;286:548-552.
Eriksson  PSPerfilieva  EBjork-Eriksson  T  et al Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med.1998;4:1313-1317.
Johansson  CBSvensson  MWallstedt  LJanson  AMFrisen  J Neural stem cells in the adult human brain. Exp Cell Res.1999;253:733-736.
Katzman  R Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.1993;43:13-20.
Katzman  RAronson  MFuld  P  et al Development of dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer cohort. Ann Neurol.1989;25:317-324.
Ince  P Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicenter community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet.2001;357:169-175.
Goldman  WPrice  JLStorandt  M  et al Absence of cognitive impairement or decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.2001;56:361-367.

Figures

Place holder to copy figure label and caption

Statistical parametric map and its 3-dimensional brain rendering representation depicting areas of significant (some P<.001, some P<.01, and some P<.05, as presented in Table 4) inverse correlations between cerebral blood flow and activities score in the Alzheimer group, controlling for age and Mini-Mental State Examination score.

Graphic Jump Location

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1 Items Included in the Activities Scale
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2 Demographic, Clinical, and Neuropsychologic Data for 9 Patients With Alzheimer Disease
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3 Demographic, Cognitive, and Functional Characteristics of the Subjects*
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 4 Local Maxima With Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow (CBF) and Education and Between CBF and Nelson Adult Reading Test Score*
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 5 Local Maxima With a Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow and Activity Scores*
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 6 Local Maxima With a Statistically Significant Inverse Association Between Cerebral Blood Flow and Activity Scores*

References

Stern  Y What is cognitive reserve? theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.2002;8:448-460.
Stern  YGurland  BTatemichi  TKTang  MXWilder  DMayeux  R Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. JAMA.1994;271:1004-1010.
McGeer  PLKamo  HHarrop  R  et al Positron emission tomography in patients with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. CMAJ.1986;134:597-607.
McGeer  PLKamo  HHarrop  R  et al Comparison of PET, MRI, and CT with pathology in a proven case of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.1986;36:1569-1574.
McGeer  EGMcGeer  PLHarrop  RAkiyama  HKamo  H Correlations of regional postmortem enzyme activities with premortem local glucose metabolic rates in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci Res.1990;27:612-619.
McGeer  EGPeppard  RPMcGeer  PL  et al 18Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography studies in presumed Alzheimer cases, including 13 serial scans. Can J Neurol Sci.1990;17:1-11.
Mielke  RSchroder  RFink  GRKessler  JHerholz  KHeiss  WD Regional cerebral glucose metabolism and postmortem pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neuropathol.1996;91:174-179.
Hoffman  JMWelsh-Bohmer  KAHanson  M  et al FDG PET imaging in patients with pathologically verified dementia. J Nucl Med.2000;41:1920-1928.
Friedland  RPBrun  ABudinger  TF Pathological and positron emission tomographic correlations in Alzheimer's disease [letter]. Lancet.1985;1:228.
DeCarli  CAtack  JRBall  MJ  et al Post-mortem regional neurofibrillary tangle densities but not senile plaque densities are related to regional cerebral metabolic rates for glucose during life in Alzheimer's disease patients. Neurodegeneration.1992;1:113-121.
Stern  YAlexander  GEProhovnik  IMayeux  R Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol.1992;32:371-375.
Stern  YAlexander  GEProhovnik  I  et al Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: implications for a reserve against Alzheimer's disease pathology. Neurology.1995;45:55-60.
Alexander  GEFurey  MLGrady  CL  et al Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry.1997;154:165-172.
Kondo  KNiino  MShido  K A case-control study of Alzheimer's disease in Japan: significance of life-styles. Dementia.1994;5:314-326.
Friedland  RPFritsch  TSmyth  KA  et al Patients with Alzheimer's disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.2001;98:3440-3445.
Fabrigoule  CLetenneur  LDartigues  JFZarrouk  MCommenges  DBarberger-Gateau  P Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc.1995;43:485-490.
Wilson  RSMendes de Leon  CFBarnes  L  et al Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA.2002;287:742-748.
Scarmeas  NLevy  GTang  MManly  JStern  Y Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.2001;57:2236-2242.
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.  Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987.
McKhann  GDrachman  DFolstein  MKatzman  RPrice  DStadlan  EM Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology.1984;34:939-944.
Wechsler  D Wechler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised.  New York, NY: The Psychological Corp; 1981.
Grober  ESliwinski  M Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.1991;13:933-949.
Nelson  HEO'Connell  A Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex.1978;14:234-244.
Nelson  HE The National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual.  Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson; 1982.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res.1975;12:189-198.
Mayeux  RStern  YRosen  JLeventhal  J Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson disease. Neurology.1981;31:645-650.
Stern  YSano  MPaulson  JMayeux  R Modified mini-mental state examination: validity and reliability. Neurology.1987;37 (suppl 1):179.
Buschke  HFuld  PA Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology.1974;24:1019-1025.
Benton  A The Visual Retention Test.  New York, NY: The Psychological Corp; 1955.
Mattis  S Mental Status ecamination for organic mental syndrome inthe elderly patient.  In: Bellak  L Karasu TB, eds. Geriatric Psychiatry. New York, NY: Grune & Stratton; 1976.
Kaplan  EGoodglass  HWeintraub  S Boston Naming Test.  Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
Benton  AHamsher  A Multiligual Aphasia Examination.  Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1976.
Goodglass  HKaplan  D The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa: Lea & Febiger; 1983.
Not Available The Rosen Drawing Test.  Bronx NY: Veterans Administration Medical Center; 1981.
Blessed  GTomlinson  BERoth  M The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry.1968;114:797-811.
Arnold  SEHyman  BTFlory  JDamasio  ARVan Hoesen  GW The topographical and neuroanatomical distribution of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in the cerebral cortex of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cereb Cortex.1991;1:103-116.
Smith  GSde Leon  MJGeorge  AE  et al Topography of cross-sectional and longitudinal glucose metabolic deficits in Alzheimer's disease: pathophysiologic implications. Arch Neurol.1992;49:1142-1150.
Friston  KJHolmes  APWorsley  KJPoline  JPFrackowiak  RSJ Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp.1995;2:189-210.
Lezak  M Neuropsychological Assessment.  New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1995.
Stern  YSano  MPaulson  JMayeux  R Modified mini-mental state examination: validity and reliability. Neurology.1987;37(suppl 1):179.
Cabeza  R Functional neuroimaging of cognitive aging.  In: Cabeza  R, Kingstone  A, eds.Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition. Cambridge, Mass: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press; 2001:331-377.
Worsley  KJMarrett  SNeelin  PVandal  ACFriston  KJEvans  AC A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp.1996;4:58-73.
Hultsch  DFHertzog  CSmall  BJDixon  RA Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging.1999;14:245-263.
Gold  DPAndres  DEtezadi  JArbuckle  TSchwartzman  AChaikelson  J Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. Psychol Aging.1995;10:294-303.
Schaie  K Midlife influences upon intellectual functioning in old age. Int J Behav Dev.1984;7:463-478.
Li  GShen  YCChen  CHZhau  YWLi  SRLu  M A three-year follow-up study of age-related dementia in an urban area of Beijing. Acta Psychiatr Scand.1991;83:99-104.
Yoshitake  TKiyohara  YKato  I  et al Incidence and risk factors of vascular dementia and Alzheimer's disease in a defined elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Neurology.1995;45:1161-1168.
Laurin  DVerreault  RLindsay  JMacPherson  KRockwood  K Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly persons. Arch Neurol.2001;58:498-504.
Broe  GACreasey  HJorm  AF  et al Health habits and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in old age: a prospective study on the effects of exercise, smoking and alcohol consumption. Aust N Z J Public Health.1998;22:621-623.
van Praag  HKempermann  GGage  FH Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci.1999;2:266-270.
Rogers  RLMeyer  JSMortel  KF After reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc.1990;38:123-128.
Dustman  RERuhling  RORussell  EM  et al Aerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individuals. Neurobiol Aging.1984;5:35-42.
Spirduso  WW Physical fitness, aging, and psychomotor speed: a review. J Gerontol.1980;35:850-865.
Kempermann  GKuhn  HGGage  FH More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature.1997;386:493-495.
Gould  EReeves  AJGraziano  MSGross  CG Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science.1999;286:548-552.
Eriksson  PSPerfilieva  EBjork-Eriksson  T  et al Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med.1998;4:1313-1317.
Johansson  CBSvensson  MWallstedt  LJanson  AMFrisen  J Neural stem cells in the adult human brain. Exp Cell Res.1999;253:733-736.
Katzman  R Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.1993;43:13-20.
Katzman  RAronson  MFuld  P  et al Development of dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer cohort. Ann Neurol.1989;25:317-324.
Ince  P Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicenter community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet.2001;357:169-175.
Goldman  WPrice  JLStorandt  M  et al Absence of cognitive impairement or decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurology.2001;56:361-367.

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Web of Science® Times Cited: 101

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles
JAMAevidence.com

Users' Guides to the Medical Literature
Clinical Resolution

Users' Guides to the Medical Literature
Clinical Scenario