0
We're unable to sign you in at this time. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
We were able to sign you in, but your subscription(s) could not be found. Please try again in a few minutes.
Retry
There may be a problem with your account. Please contact the AMA Service Center to resolve this issue.
Contact the AMA Service Center:
Telephone: 1 (800) 262-2350 or 1 (312) 670-7827  *   Email: subscriptions@jamanetwork.com
Error Message ......
Original Contribution |

Outcomes of Mild Cognitive Impairment by Definition:  A Population Study FREE

Mary Ganguli, MD, MPH; Beth E. Snitz, PhD; Judith A. Saxton, PhD; Chung-Chou H. Chang, PhD; Ching-Wen Lee, MS; Joni Vander Bilt, MPH; Tiffany F. Hughes, PhD; David A. Loewenstein, PhD; Frederick W. Unverzagt, PhD; Ronald C. Petersen, PhD, MD
[+] Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Ganguli and Hughes and Mss Lee and Vander Bilt), Neurology (Drs Ganguli, Snitz, and Saxton), and Medicine (Dr Chang), University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; Departments of Epidemiology (Dr Ganguli) and Biostatistics (Dr Chang and Ms Lee), University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health; Department of Psychiatry, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, Florida (Dr Loewenstein); Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis (Dr Unverzagt); and Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic College of Medicine, Rochester, Minnesota (Dr Petersen).


Arch Neurol. 2011;68(6):761-767. doi:10.1001/archneurol.2011.101.
Text Size: A A A
Published online

Background  Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) has been defined in several ways.

Objective  To determine the 1-year outcomes of MCI by different definitions at the population level.

Design  Inception cohort with 1-year follow-up. Participants were classified as having MCI using the following definitions operationalized for this study: amnestic MCI by Mayo criteria, expanded MCI by International Working Group criteria, Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) = 0.5, and a purely cognitive classification into amnestic and nonamnestic MCI.

Setting  General community.

Participants  Stratified random population-based sample of 1982 individuals 65 years and older.

Main Outcome Measures  For each MCI definition, there were 3 possible outcomes: worsening (progression to dementia [CDR ≥ 1] or severe cognitive impairment), improvement (reversion to CDR = 0 or normal cognition), and stability (unchanged CDR or cognitive status).

Results  Regardless of MCI definition, over 1 year, a small proportion of participants progressed to CDR > 1 (range, 0%-3%) or severe cognitive impairment (0%-20%) at rates higher than their cognitively normal peers. Somewhat larger proportions of participants improved or reverted to normal (6%-53%). Most participants remained stable (29%-92%). Where definitions focused on memory impairment and on multiple cognitive domains, higher proportions progressed and lower proportions reverted on the CDR.

Conclusions  As ascertained by several operational definitions, MCI is a heterogeneous entity at the population level but progresses to dementia at rates higher than in normal elderly individuals. Proportions of participants progressing to dementia are lower and proportions reverting to normal are higher than in clinical populations. Memory impairments and impairments in multiple domains lead to greater progression and lesser improvement. Research criteria may benefit from validation at the community level before incorporation into clinical practice.

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the cognitive state intermediate between normal cognition and dementia, is interesting because of the potential for MCI to eventually develop into full-blown dementia. The original Mayo Clinic criteria for amnestic MCI1 focused on deficits in and complaints about memory. The revised International Working Group (IWG) on Mild Cognitive Impairment criteria2 expanded the concept to include objective and subjective impairments in any of several cognitive domains. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR)3 approach focuses on decline in cognitively driven everyday function rather than on objective cognitive deficits. Other approaches have used purely neuropsychological definitions of MCI based on objective deficits relative to norms.47 Regardless of the definition used, individuals with MCI progress to dementia in higher proportions than do cognitively normal people; however, the actual proportions vary across definitions4,5,810 and across operational versions of the same definitions.1113 Rates of progression from MCI to dementia are consistently lower in community settings than in specialty clinical and research programs where individuals with MCI seek services11,12 despite using the same criteria.14 All population-based studies4,8,1012 find substantial proportions of individuals with variously defined MCI remaining stable or even reverting to normal during follow-up.

In a population-based cohort study, we collected data sufficient to apply operational versions of several different MCI definitions simultaneously. Having previously showed that the prevalence of MCI varies considerably depending on the definition used,15 we now report 1-year outcomes of MCI according to these multiple definitions.

STUDY AREA, SAMPLING, AND RECRUITMENT

The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team study cohort was an age-stratified random sample of the population aged 65 years and older drawn from the publicly available voter registration list for a small-town region of Pennsylvania. Community outreach, recruitment, and assessment protocols were approved by the University of Pittsburgh institutional review board for the protection of human subjects.1517 The recruitment criteria were (1) age 65 years or older, (2) living in the selected towns, and (3) not already in long-term care institutions. Individuals were ineligible if they (1) were too ill to participate, (2) had severe vision or hearing impairments, or (3) were decisionally incapacitated. In a 2-year period, 2036 individuals were recruited.

ASSESSMENT AND CLASSIFICATION (OVERVIEW)

Trained interviewers obtained written informed consent and administered the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)18 and scored it immediately,16 applying a standard correction for age and education.19 We classified 54 individuals (2.7%) scoring less than 21 of 30 (age and education corrected) as moderately or severely impaired and, thus, unsuited to a study of mild impairment; we did not assess them further. Having designated the remaining 1982 participants as cognitively normal or only mildly impaired, we immediately performed detailed assessments15,17; a year later, we invited the participants to undergo repeated assessments.20 At each assessment, we classified participants according to several criteria for MCI: the CDR, a purely cognitive classification,20 and the standard Mayo1 and IWG2 criteria. All the results reported herein refer to our operational definitions of these criteria.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT

We assessed cognitive functioning using a comprehensive test battery17,20 on which we have reported population-based norms17 also available on the study Web site (http://www.wpic.pitt.edu/research/dementia_epidemiology/MYHAT/MYHATHomePage.htm). We categorized these tests according to the principal cognitive domains that they tap (attention/processing speed, executive function, language, memory, and visuospatial function) and created a composite score for each domain.17

CLINICAL DEMENTIA RATING

Appropriately certified21 interviewers rated participants on the CDR scale3 using an assessment protocol composed of standardized questions and observations regarding everyday functioning in the 6 areas of memory, orientation, judgment, home and hobbies, community affairs, and personal care. Most of these normal or only mildly impaired older adults provided their own self-report information and did not have surrogate informants; thus, this CDR may be considered a field adaptation for population settings. The CDR for each participant was finalized by consensus among 2 or more interviewers, ignoring the neuropsychological data but determining that the reported or observed functional impairments were attributable to cognitive rather than, for example, sensory or motor difficulties.

COGNITIVE CLASSIFICATION

To complement the purely functionally based CDR, we developed a solely neuropsychologically based cognitive classification.15 Based on normative reference points,17 we classified participants as follows: (1) cognitively normal: composite scores in all domains within 1.0 SD of the mean for the individual's age/sex/education group; (2) severe cognitive impairment: composite scores in at least 2 domains 2.0 SDs below the mean for the appropriate reference group; (3) MCI single domain: composite score in 1 domain greater than 1.0 SD below the mean, with all other domains in the normal range; or (4) MCI multidomain: composite scores in 2 or more domains 1.0 to 2.0 SDs below the appropriate mean or no more than 1 domain composite greater than 2.0 SDs below the mean with other domains 1.0 to 2.0 SDs below the mean. We also explored a “narrow” variation of these definitions, using a more stringent MCI threshold of 1.5 SDs rather than 1.0 SD below the mean, eliminating those with scores between 1.0 and 1.5 SDs.

STANDARD MCI CRITERIA

For the Mayo criteria for amnestic MCI1 and the expanded IWG criteria,2 we operationalized the objective cognitive deficit criteria as in the cognitive classification described previously herein (the memory domain alone for Mayo MCI and any domain for IWG MCI). For subjective reports and concerns, we operationalized the Mayo criteria as at least 2 items (median score) endorsed from a list of 16 “remembering” questions; for the IWG criteria, we included an additional 5 questions on “nonremembering” functions. We defined absence of dementia as CDR < 1, essentially normal daily functioning as no impaired instrumental activities of daily living on the Older Americans Resources and Services scale,22 and essentially normal mental status as an MMSE score of at least 21. Details have been reported previously.15

Because the original Mayo criteria1 did not specify that domains other than memory could be impaired, we operationalized them as requiring isolated (single-domain) memory deficit. Subsequently, we also explored a multidomain definition, having been advised by a coauthor (R.C.P.) that the Mayo group included individuals with impairments in additional domains. We also examined the effects of (1) raising the MMSE score threshold from at least 21 at the 5th percentile of the original cohort to greater than 23 at its 10th percentile, thus eliminating those with scores of 21 to 23, and (2) lowering the cognitive composite threshold from 1.0 to 1.5 SDs below the appropriate mean. Because both of these changes reduce the number of people classified as having MCI, we refer to them as “narrow versions.”

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

For each baseline MCI group, we identified 2 outcome variables at follow-up: CDR and the cognitive classification. For each outcome, we examined 3 possibilities:

  • Worsening: progression to CDR ≥ 1 (dementia) in individuals with baseline CDR < 1 or severe cognitive impairment (defined previously herein) in persons with baseline cognitive classification of normal or MCI.

  • Improvement: reversion to CDR = 0 (no dementia) in those with baseline CDR = 0.5 or normal cognition (defined previously herein) in those with baseline cognitive classification of MCI.

  • Stability: no change in CDR or cognitive classification.

We compared proportions of participants experiencing these 6 outcomes using the 1-sample proportion z test at P < .05. This test allows proportions to be compared between nonindependent (overlapping) samples by treating 1 of the samples as though it were the population from which the other sample was drawn.23 Finally, we compared the characteristics of participants assessed at follow-up with those of participants lost to follow-up.

At baseline, all 1982 cohort members with normal or mildly impaired cognition (MMSE score ≥21) were rated on the CDR; 1941 participants had sufficient neuropsychological data to be classified by the cognitive definitions; 1950 and 1957 participants provided sufficient cognitive, functional (instrumental activities of daily living), and subjective report data to be classified by the Mayo1 and IWG2 criteria, respectively. Proportions of participants meeting these criteria, corresponding to the published prevalence estimates,15 and their demographic characteristics were identified (Table 1). Note that the MCI definitions overlap: the Mayo criteria are a subset of the IWG criteria, and both overlap with the CDR; the 4 cognitive classification subgroups are mutually exclusive with one another but overlap with the CDR and the Mayo and IWG criteria. Of 697 individuals with MCI by the cognitive classification, 433 (62.1%) had CDR = 0; most of these participants (n = 316) had nonamnestic single-domain impairment. Of 45 participants meeting the Mayo criteria, 13 (28.9%) had CDR = 0; of 350 participants meeting the IWG criteria, 143 (40.9%) had CDR = 0. As expected, the narrow versions reduced baseline prevalence for the standard and cognitive criteria. Some individuals were classified as having MCI under the original versions and as normal under the narrow versions; others originally classified as having multidomain MCI were reclassified as having single-domain MCI.

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Frequencies, Proportions, and Demographic Characteristics of Cohort Subgroups at Baseline and at the First Annual Follow-up Assessment

Participants who met the operationalized Mayo criteria were significantly younger than were those with CDR = 0.5, were more likely to be men, and had more education than did those classified by the IWG criteria, by CDR = 0.5, and by the cognitive classification. Those fulfilling the IWG criteria were significantly younger and better educated than were those with CDR = 0.5. The narrow versions of the criteria had minimal effects on the demographics of MCI (Table 1).

At 1-year follow-up, 1697 of the original 1982 cohort members (85.6%) underwent repeated assessment. The total attrition of 14.4% represented 3% from mortality and 11.4% from other causes, including relocation, illness, and elective dropout. The 285 attrited individuals were significantly older (78.7 vs 77.5 years, P < .02) and were less likely to be women (55.1% vs 62.1%, P < .03) than were those who were seen at follow-up, with no significant difference in educational level (16.8% and 13.3% with less than a high school education).20 Compared with participants who were reassessed, those lost to follow-up had slightly but significantly lower mean (SD) baseline MMSE scores (26.4 [2.7] vs 27.0 [2.4]), had a lower proportion with baseline CDR = 0 (60.0% vs 73.2% %), and were significantly more likely to be classified at baseline by the CDR and all the cognitive definitions except multidomain amnestic MCI (P < .001 for all).

1-YEAR CDR OUTCOMES

Progression to CDR ≥ 1 ranged from 0% for purely cognitive nonamnestic multidomain MCI to 2.8% for the Mayo criteria, noting that the latter represents only 1 participant. Reversion to CDR = 0 ranged from 10.8% in those with baseline amnestic multidomain MCI (and CDR = 0.5) to 26.5% in those with nonamnestic single-domain MCI (and CDR = 0.5). The stability of the CDR was uniformly high across groups, ranging from 72% for CDR = 0.5 and the Mayo criteria and 78% for the purely cognitive amnestic single-domain MCI to 80% to 88% for all other definitions (Table 2).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. One-Year Outcomes of MCI Defined by CDR3,a
1-YEAR COGNITIVE OUTCOMES

Progression to severe cognitive impairment ranged from 1.1% for purely cognitive nonamnestic single-domain MCI to 19.8% for amnestic multidomain MCI. Reversion to cognitively normal ranged from 6.3% for purely cognitive amnestic multidomain MCI to 53.4% for nonamnestic single-domain MCI. Cognitive classification stability ranged from 28.6% for the nonamnestic single-domain category to 54.5% for CDR = 0.5 (Table 3).

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3. One-Year Outcomes of MCI Defined by Cognitive Classification15,a
VARYING THE DEFINITIONS

Expanding the Mayo MCI criteria by allowing impairment in additional domains besides memory increased baseline prevalence from 2.27% to 6.21%, that is, 123 individuals (of whom 96 completed follow-up). Seventy of these participants had baseline CDR = 0.5, of whom 1 (1.4%) progressed to CDR ≥ 1 at follow-up, 8 (11.4%) reverted to CDR = 0, and 61 (87.1%) remained at CDR = 0.5. Of 26 participants with baseline CDR = 0, 7 (26.9%) progressed to CDR = 0.5 and none to CDR ≥ 1, whereas 19 (73.1%) were stable at CDR = 0. Overall, progression from a lower to a higher CDR level was seen in 8 individuals (8.3%).

Narrowing the definitions by raising the MMSE score threshold to 24 (Mayo and IWG criteria) and by lowering the cognitive threshold to 1.5 SDs below the appropriate mean (Mayo, IWG, and cognitive classification) in reducing prevalence also reduced the denominator for calculating proportions with the different outcomes (eTable 1 and eTable 2). Although the actual numbers were mostly too small to be compared for statistical significance, the overall pattern was that the narrow definitions very slightly increased the proportions that worsened, reduced the proportions that improved on CDR, reduced sensitivity, and increased specificity.

ATTRITION EFFECTS: SENSITIVITY ANALYSES

The actual outcomes of those lost to follow-up are not knowable. In the worst case, if all attrited participants with baseline MCI had remained in the study and worsened at follow-up, the overall proportions that worsened to CDR ≥ 1 (range, 15.0%-25.8%) or severe cognitive impairment (range, 16.7%-39.9%) would be significantly increased for all MCI definitions except multidomain nonamnestic MCI. Note that these percentages represent small numbers.

In a randomly sampled population-based cohort, the baseline prevalence of MCI varied by definition,15 as would be expected with any syndromic entity. During 1-year follow-up, the same MCI definitions led to different proportions of progression (worsening), reversion (improvement), and stability. Because the various criterion sets overlap only partially, individuals can be classified as impaired by one definition and as normal by another. For example, with the Mayo criteria defined as including CDR < 1, individuals with MCI could be either CDR = 0 or CDR = 0.5; among these, only those at CDR = 0.5 can revert to CDR = 0. Similarly, by designating worsening as progression to CDR ≥ 1, we could be underestimating progression by not including worsening from CDR = 0 to CDR = 0.5.

With the previous caveats, across MCI definitions, relatively small proportions of participants (0%-3%) progressed to dementia defined as CDR ≥ 1 but at significantly higher rates than did those with normal cognition. Progression was most likely to be seen in the definitions that emphasized memory. Larger proportions of participants (up to 20%) were found to worsen when the outcome was defined as severe cognitive impairment; again, MCI definitions with amnestic components showed the highest progression. In the purely cognitive classification, multidomain amnestic impairment showed more progression than did single-domain amnestic impairment, whereas nonamnestic impairments had the lowest progression.

Greater proportions of individuals with baseline MCI showed improvement compared with worsening at 1-year follow-up. With CDR = 0 as the outcome, the range of reversion was approximately a quarter of those with baseline CDR = 0.5 alone. When reversion was characterized as normal cognition, the lowest proportion reverted from amnestic multidomain MCI (which also had the highest proportion progressing to severe cognitive impairment). Although reversion in the purely cognitive definition could be attributed to instability of measurement or intraindividual variability, the same phenomenon was also observed for the functional (CDR) and combined cognitive-functional (Mayo and IWG) definitions.

However, the most frequent outcome was stability, that is, no change in MCI status. These proportions ranged from 72% to 88% for stable CDR and from 29% to 55% for stable MCI by broad cognitive classification. The single-domain MCIs, whether amnestic or nonamnestic, were the least likely to remain cognitively unchanged, whereas the CDR = 0.5 group was the most likely to remain cognitively unchanged.

The present cohort study indicates, as have previous studies,4,8,1012 that the MCI syndrome is a heterogeneous entity at the population level regardless of definition. A small proportion progresses to dementia or severe cognitive impairment, a somewhat larger proportion reverts to normal, and most remain unchanged. Most individuals with mild impairment remain at least mildly impaired. Some individuals who meet MCI criteria may have always functioned at a mildly impaired level, invoking the concept of “accidental MCI.”24 The heterogeneity in outcome observed herein is greater than that reported from clinical settings, where individuals seek care for cognitive impairment and where progression occurs at a rate of 12% to 15% annually.13

Heterogeneity in outcome suggests heterogeneity in the underlying pathology. Definitions of MCI that reflect impairment in memory predicted more progression and less reversion than did impairment in other domains, regardless of whether the definition was neuropsychological, functional, or both. Because memory deficits are the hallmark of dementing disorders, such as Alzheimer disease, MCI definitions centered on memory may be identifying individuals in the early stages of these disorders, which may be in the minority at the population level.

Finally, the present data indicate that mild impairment in 1 domain, memory or otherwise, is more likely to predict reversion to normal than is mild impairment in 2 or more domains, as was also observed in another population study.25 This source of variation could represent heterogeneity in etiology or in stage along the course of a given disease. For example, an individual with a nonprogressive condition, such as depression, medication adverse effects, or hypoxia, might experience transient or reversible cognitive impairment that reflects resolution of the underlying condition. Alternatively, a person at a very early stage of Alzheimer disease might not manifest cognitive deficits unless a second condition is also present; if the second condition resolves, improvement might be noted temporarily. Furthermore, a person with any progressive dementia may experience lability or “wobble” in function and performance early in the disease course, before the deficits become more pervasive and sustained, and this intraindividual variability itself may reflect underlying brain disease.26 Compared with single-domain impairments, impairment in 2 domains suggests a greater likelihood of underlying disease, which has reached a later stage, closer to the dementia threshold. Even in the normal range of cognition, poorer neuropsychological test scores predict subsequent decline in CDR.20 In the present cohort, as in clinical practice, a minority of individuals did not return for follow-up assessment at 1 year because of death, relocation, illness, or elective dropout. At study entry, these individuals were older, less educated, and more cognitively impaired than were those who were followed up. Had they remained in the study and experienced worsening in large enough proportions, they may have increased the progression rates for most but not all MCI definitions.

Although 1 year is a relatively short period in which to observe MCI outcomes, it mirrors clinical practice wherein the first annual follow-up is often essential to validate the original diagnosis. Although longer prospective studies usually report annual progression rates averaged over multiple years,5,8,12,14 the annual rate changes over time.12 Repeated assessments of this cohort over a longer follow-up period will clarify these patterns and identify the profiles of individuals whose mild impairments are likely to develop into dementia eventually. The incorporation of biomarker and risk factor assessments may further improve the characterization of MCI at the population level.

Population-based cohorts experience less selection bias and are more representative of the community than are specialty clinic samples. Being large, they are powered to detect relatively small effects. However, participants recruited randomly from the community are not necessarily concerned about their cognition; their subjective reports (“complaints”) are not spontaneously offered but rather are elicited by standardized questions and may vary in clinical and prognostic significance. Normal or mildly impaired individuals in the community who are not seeking care for cognitive difficulties may not have surrogate informants more knowledgeable than themselves about their own everyday functioning. Dementia ratings based on participants' self-reports plus raters' observations may differ from ratings based on family reports typically obtained in specialty clinics. In most population studies, participants are assessed using standardized protocols implemented by research personnel who, although highly trained, are not expert clinicians exercising judgment regarding help-seeking patients in the clinic. Classification is based on statistical or actuarial criteria, which in clinical settings could be overridden by expert clinical judgment. Thus, heterogeneity in the participant pool and methodological factors can account for variance in proportions with progression and reversion in MCI between clinical and community studies, with results fairly consistent in these groups of studies.27 Population-based data illustrate the importance of validating research criteria at the community level before they are recommended for clinical practice.

Correspondence: Mary Ganguli, MD, MPH, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, 3811 O’Hara St, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (gangulim@upmc.edu).

Accepted for Publication: November 8, 2010.

Author Contributions: All authors had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Ganguli, Snitz, and Saxton. Acquisition of data: Ganguli, Saxton, Chang, and Vander Bilt. Analysis and interpretation of data: Ganguli, Snitz, Saxton, Chang, Lee, Vander Bilt, Hughes, Loewenstein, Unverzagt, and Petersen. Drafting of the manuscript: Ganguli. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Ganguli, Snitz, Saxton, Chang, Lee, Vander Bilt, Hughes, Loewenstein, Unverzagt, and Petersen. Statistical analysis: Chang and Lee. Obtained funding: Ganguli and Saxton. Administrative, technical, and material support: Ganguli, Saxton, and Vander Bilt. Study supervision: Ganguli, Snitz, and Saxton.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported in part by grants R01AG023651, P30AG005133, and K24AG022035 from the National Institute on Aging, US Department of Health and Human Services.

Additional Contributions: John C. Morris, MD, Washington University in St Louis, provided input into the CDR scoring rules. We appreciate the efforts and contributions of all Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team staff and the gracious cooperation of all the participants.

Petersen  RCSmith  GEWaring  SCIvnik  RJTangalos  EGKokmen  E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56 (3) 303- 308
PubMed Link to Article
Winblad  BPalmer  KKivipelto  M  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med 2004;256 (3) 240- 246
PubMed Link to Article
Morris  JC The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43 (11) 2412- 2414
PubMed Link to Article
Saxton  JSnitz  BELopez  OL  et al. GEM Study Investigators, Functional and cognitive criteria produce different rates of mild cognitive impairment and conversion to dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009;80 (7) 737- 743
PubMed Link to Article
Jak  AJBondi  MWDelano-Wood  L  et al.  Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17 (5) 368- 375
PubMed Link to Article
Loewenstein  DAAcevedo  ASmall  BJAgron  JCrocco  EDuara  R Stability of different subtypes of mild cognitive impairment among the elderly over a 2- to 3-year follow-up period. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;27 (5) 418- 423
PubMed Link to Article
Kochan  NASlavin  MJBrodaty  H  et al.  Effect of different impairment criteria on prevalence of “objective” mild cognitive impairment in a community sample [published online ahead of print May 2010]. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010;18 (8) 711- 722
Link to Article
Matthews  FEStephan  BCM McKeith  IGBond  JBrayne  CMedical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study, Two-year progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia: to what extent do different definitions agree? J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56 (8) 1424- 1433
PubMed Link to Article
Visser  PJVerhey  FRJ Mild cognitive impairment as predictor for Alzheimer's disease in clinical practice: effect of age and diagnostic criteria. Psychol Med 2008;38 (1) 113- 122
PubMed Link to Article
Palmer  KBäckman  LWinblad  BFratiglioni  L Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;16 (7) 603- 611
PubMed Link to Article
Bruscoli  MLovestone  S Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of conversion studies. Int Psychogeriatr 2004;16 (2) 129- 140
PubMed Link to Article
Mitchell  AJShiri-Feshki  M Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia—meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;119 (4) 252- 265
PubMed Link to Article
Petersen  RCRoberts  ROKnopman  DS  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol 2009;66 (12) 1447- 1455
PubMed Link to Article
Farias  STMungas  DReed  BRHarvey  DDeCarli  C Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs community-based cohorts. Arch Neurol 2009;66 (9) 1151- 1157
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MChang  CCSnitz  BESaxton  JAVander Bilt  JLee  CW Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010;18 (8) 674- 683
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MSnitz  BVander Bilt  JChang  CC-H How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;24 (11) 1277- 1284
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MSnitz  BELee  CWVander Bilt  JSaxton  JAChang  CCH Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team. Aging Ment Health 2010;14 (1) 100- 107
PubMed Link to Article
Folstein  MFFolstein  SE McHugh  PR “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12 (3) 189- 198
PubMed Link to Article
Mungas  DMarshall  SCWeldon  MHaan  MReed  BR Age and education correction of Mini-Mental State Examination for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology 1996;46 (3) 700- 706
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MVander Bilt  JVLee  C-W  et al.  Cognitive test performance predicts change in functional status at the population level: the MYHAT Project. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16 (5) 761- 770
PubMed Link to Article
Washington University in St. Louis, Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Clinical Dementia Rating, on-line training. http://alzheimer.wustl.edu/cdr/default.htmAccessed March 2006
Fillenbaum  GG  Multidimensional Functional Assessment of Older Adults: The Duke Older Americans Resources and Services Procedures.   Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc1988;
Rosner  B  Fundamentals of Biostatistics.  6th ed. Pacific Grove, CA Duxbury Press2006;
Brooks  BLIverson  GLWhite  T Substantial risk of “accidental MCI” in healthy older adults: base rates of low memory scores in neuropsychological assessment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13 (3) 490- 500
PubMed Link to Article
Manly  JJTang  MXSchupf  NStern  YVonsattel  JPMayeux  R Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann Neurol 2008;63 (4) 494- 506
PubMed Link to Article
Hultsch  DFMacDonald  SWSHunter  MALevy-Bencheton  JStrauss  E Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology 2000;14 (4) 588- 598
PubMed Link to Article
Petersen  RC Does the source of subjects matter? absolutely! Neurology 2010;74 (22) 1754- 1755
PubMed Link to Article

Figures

Tables

Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 1. Frequencies, Proportions, and Demographic Characteristics of Cohort Subgroups at Baseline and at the First Annual Follow-up Assessment
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 2. One-Year Outcomes of MCI Defined by CDR3,a
Table Graphic Jump LocationTable 3. One-Year Outcomes of MCI Defined by Cognitive Classification15,a

References

Petersen  RCSmith  GEWaring  SCIvnik  RJTangalos  EGKokmen  E Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56 (3) 303- 308
PubMed Link to Article
Winblad  BPalmer  KKivipelto  M  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med 2004;256 (3) 240- 246
PubMed Link to Article
Morris  JC The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43 (11) 2412- 2414
PubMed Link to Article
Saxton  JSnitz  BELopez  OL  et al. GEM Study Investigators, Functional and cognitive criteria produce different rates of mild cognitive impairment and conversion to dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009;80 (7) 737- 743
PubMed Link to Article
Jak  AJBondi  MWDelano-Wood  L  et al.  Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17 (5) 368- 375
PubMed Link to Article
Loewenstein  DAAcevedo  ASmall  BJAgron  JCrocco  EDuara  R Stability of different subtypes of mild cognitive impairment among the elderly over a 2- to 3-year follow-up period. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;27 (5) 418- 423
PubMed Link to Article
Kochan  NASlavin  MJBrodaty  H  et al.  Effect of different impairment criteria on prevalence of “objective” mild cognitive impairment in a community sample [published online ahead of print May 2010]. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010;18 (8) 711- 722
Link to Article
Matthews  FEStephan  BCM McKeith  IGBond  JBrayne  CMedical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study, Two-year progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia: to what extent do different definitions agree? J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56 (8) 1424- 1433
PubMed Link to Article
Visser  PJVerhey  FRJ Mild cognitive impairment as predictor for Alzheimer's disease in clinical practice: effect of age and diagnostic criteria. Psychol Med 2008;38 (1) 113- 122
PubMed Link to Article
Palmer  KBäckman  LWinblad  BFratiglioni  L Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;16 (7) 603- 611
PubMed Link to Article
Bruscoli  MLovestone  S Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of conversion studies. Int Psychogeriatr 2004;16 (2) 129- 140
PubMed Link to Article
Mitchell  AJShiri-Feshki  M Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia—meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;119 (4) 252- 265
PubMed Link to Article
Petersen  RCRoberts  ROKnopman  DS  et al.  Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol 2009;66 (12) 1447- 1455
PubMed Link to Article
Farias  STMungas  DReed  BRHarvey  DDeCarli  C Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs community-based cohorts. Arch Neurol 2009;66 (9) 1151- 1157
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MChang  CCSnitz  BESaxton  JAVander Bilt  JLee  CW Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010;18 (8) 674- 683
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MSnitz  BVander Bilt  JChang  CC-H How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;24 (11) 1277- 1284
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MSnitz  BELee  CWVander Bilt  JSaxton  JAChang  CCH Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team. Aging Ment Health 2010;14 (1) 100- 107
PubMed Link to Article
Folstein  MFFolstein  SE McHugh  PR “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12 (3) 189- 198
PubMed Link to Article
Mungas  DMarshall  SCWeldon  MHaan  MReed  BR Age and education correction of Mini-Mental State Examination for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology 1996;46 (3) 700- 706
PubMed Link to Article
Ganguli  MVander Bilt  JVLee  C-W  et al.  Cognitive test performance predicts change in functional status at the population level: the MYHAT Project. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16 (5) 761- 770
PubMed Link to Article
Washington University in St. Louis, Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Clinical Dementia Rating, on-line training. http://alzheimer.wustl.edu/cdr/default.htmAccessed March 2006
Fillenbaum  GG  Multidimensional Functional Assessment of Older Adults: The Duke Older Americans Resources and Services Procedures.   Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc1988;
Rosner  B  Fundamentals of Biostatistics.  6th ed. Pacific Grove, CA Duxbury Press2006;
Brooks  BLIverson  GLWhite  T Substantial risk of “accidental MCI” in healthy older adults: base rates of low memory scores in neuropsychological assessment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13 (3) 490- 500
PubMed Link to Article
Manly  JJTang  MXSchupf  NStern  YVonsattel  JPMayeux  R Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann Neurol 2008;63 (4) 494- 506
PubMed Link to Article
Hultsch  DFMacDonald  SWSHunter  MALevy-Bencheton  JStrauss  E Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology 2000;14 (4) 588- 598
PubMed Link to Article
Petersen  RC Does the source of subjects matter? absolutely! Neurology 2010;74 (22) 1754- 1755
PubMed Link to Article

Correspondence

CME
Meets CME requirements for:
Browse CME for all U.S. States
Accreditation Information
The American Medical Association is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education to provide continuing medical education for physicians. The AMA designates this journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM per course. Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity. Physicians who complete the CME course and score at least 80% correct on the quiz are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 CreditTM.
Note: You must get at least of the answers correct to pass this quiz.
You have not filled in all the answers to complete this quiz
The following questions were not answered:
Sorry, you have unsuccessfully completed this CME quiz with a score of
The following questions were not answered correctly:
Commitment to Change (optional):
Indicate what change(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Your quiz results:
The filled radio buttons indicate your responses. The preferred responses are highlighted
For CME Course: A Proposed Model for Initial Assessment and Management of Acute Heart Failure Syndromes
Indicate what changes(s) you will implement in your practice, if any, based on this CME course.
Submit a Comment

Multimedia

Some tools below are only available to our subscribers or users with an online account.

Web of Science® Times Cited: 49

Related Content

Customize your page view by dragging & repositioning the boxes below.

Articles Related By Topic
Related Collections
PubMed Articles